[1]
|
Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cogni- tive Neuroscience Review, 1, 21-62.
|
[2]
|
Alexopulos, T., & Ric, F. (2007). The Evaluation-behavior link: Direct and beyond valence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 1010-1016. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.017
|
[3]
|
Ambadar, Z., Schooler, J. W., & Cohn, J. F. (2005). Deciphering the enigmatic face: The importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychological Science,16, 403-410.
doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01548.x
|
[4]
|
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660.
|
[5]
|
Batty, M. & Taylor, M. J. (2003). Early processing of the six basic emotional expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 613-620.
doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00174-5
|
[6]
|
Beaupré, M. G., Cheung, N., & Hess, U. (2000). The Montreal set of facial displays of emotion [Slides]. Montreal, Canada: Department of Psychology, University of Quebec.
|
[7]
|
Berridge, K. C. (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition, 52, 106-128. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00014-9
|
[8]
|
Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 47-74.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152
|
[9]
|
Brouillet, T., Heurley, L., Martin, S., & Brouillet, D. (2010). The em- bodied cognition theory and the motor component of “yes” and “no” verbal responses. Acta Psychologica, 134, 310-317.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.003
|
[10]
|
Calvo, M. G., & Lundqvist, D. (2008). Facial expressions of Emotion (KDEF): Identification under different display-duration conditions. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 109-115.
doi:10.3758/BRM.40.1.109
|
[11]
|
Casasanto, D., & Dijkstra, K. (2010). Motor Action and Emotional Memory. Cognition, 115, 179-185.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.002
|
[12]
|
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic eva- luation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personnality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215- 224. doi:10.1177/0146167299025002007
|
[13]
|
Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2001). Defining the cortical visual sys- tems: “What”, “Where” and “How”. Acta Psychologica, 107, 43-68.
doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00021-X
|
[14]
|
Crockford, D. N., Goodyear, B., Edwards, J., Quickfall, J., & el-Gue- baly, N. (2005). Cue-induced brain activity in pathological gam- blers. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 787-795.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.037
|
[15]
|
Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psy- chonomic Society, 9, 353-356.
|
[16]
|
Detenber, B. H., Simons, R. F., & Bennet, G. C. (1998). Roll’ Em: the effects of picture motion on emotional responses. Journal of Broad- casting & Electronic Media, 42, 112-126.
doi:10.1080/08838159809364437
|
[17]
|
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psy- chologist, 48, 384-392. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.384
|
[18]
|
Ekman, P. (1999). Facial expressions. In Dalgleish, T., & Power, M. (Eds.). Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (pp. 301-320). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
|
[19]
|
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Measuring facial movement. Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior, 1, 56-75.
doi:10.1007/BF01115465
|
[20]
|
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Tomkins, S. S. (1971). Facial affect scoring technique: A first validity study. Semiotica, 3, 37-58.
doi:10.1515/semi.1971.3.1.37
|
[21]
|
Freina, L., Baroni, G., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). Emo- tive-concept nouns and motor responses: Attraction or repulsion? Memory & Cognition, 37, 493-499. doi:10.3758/MC.37.4.493
|
[22]
|
Goeleven, E., DeRaedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: A validation study. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1094-1118. doi:10.1080/02699930701626582
|
[23]
|
Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 21-26.
doi:10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8
|
[24]
|
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 87-108.
doi:10.1080/02699939508408966
|
[25]
|
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243-259.
doi:10.2307/1416950
|
[26]
|
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 202-211.
doi:10.3758/BF03212378
|
[27]
|
Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment. In J. B. Sidowski, J. H. Johnson, & T. A. Williams (Eds.), Technolo- gy in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems (pp. 119-167). Norwood, NY: Ablex. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x
|
[28]
|
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M. & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral and behavioral reac- tions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261-273.
|
[29]
|
Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & ?hman, A. (1998). The karolinska di- rected emotional face (KDEF). Stockholm: Department of Neuro- sciences Karolinska Hospital.
|
[30]
|
Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1988). Japanese and Caucasion Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE) [Slides]. San Francisco: Intercul- tural and Emotion Research Laboratory, Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University.
|
[31]
|
Méary, D., Kitromilides, E., Mazens, K., Graff, C., & Gentaz, E. (2007). Four-day-old human neonates look longer at non-biological motions of a single point-of-light. Plos ONE, 2, 186.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000186
|
[32]
|
Michotte, A., (1946). La perception de la causalité. Louvain: Publica- tions Universitaires de Louvain; Paris-Bruxelles: Erasme; Anvers- Gand-Amsterdam: Standaard-Boekhandel.
|
[33]
|
Michotte, A., 1962. Causalité, permanence et réalité phénoménales. Louvain: Publications Universitaires de Louvain.
|
[34]
|
Morris, J. S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Dif- ferential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124, 1241-1252.
doi:10.1093/brain/124.6.1241
|
[35]
|
Phaf, R. H., & Rotteveel, M. (2005). Affective modulation of recogni- tion bias. Emotion, 5, 309-318. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.309
|
[36]
|
Phaf, R. H., & Rotteveel, M. (2009). Looking at the bright side: The affective monitoring of direction. Emotion, 9, 729-733.
doi:10.1037/a0016308
|
[37]
|
Podevin, G. (2009). Influence des Mouvements Elémentaires sur les processus cognitifs emotionnels. Thèse de Doctorat, Université de Lille Nord de France UDL3.
|
[38]
|
Rimé, B., Boulanger, B., Laubin, P., Richir, M., & Stroobants, K. (1985). The perception of interpersonal emotions originated by patterns of movement. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 241-260.
doi:10.1007/BF00991830
|
[39]
|
Rimé, B., & Schiaratura, L. (1991). Gesture and speech. In R. S. Feldman, & B. Rimé (Eds.), Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior (pp. 239-281). New York: Press Syndicate of the University of Cambridge.
|
[40]
|
Rothermund, K. (2003). Motivation and attention: Incongruent effects of feedback on the processing of valence. Emotion, 3, 223-238.
doi:10.1037/1528-3542.3.3.223
|
[41]
|
Rothermund, K., Voss, A., & Wentura, D. (2008). Counter-regulation in affective attentional biases: A basic mechanism that warrants flexibility in emotion and motivation. Emotion, 8, 34-46.
doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.34
|
[42]
|
Rusinek, S. (2009). Désensibilisation de l'arachnophobie à l'aide d'une induction émotionnelle positive par le mouvement. 1er Congrès Fran?ais de Psychiatrie, Nice, décembre 2009.
|
[43]
|
Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X., & Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1153- 1172. doi:10.1080/02699930903274322
|
[44]
|
Simons, R. F., Detenber, B. H., Roedema, T. M., & Reiss, J. E. (1999). Emotion processing in three systems: The medium and the message. Psychophysiology, 36, 619-627. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3650619
|
[45]
|
Simons, R. F., Detenber, B. H., Reiss, J. E., & Shults, C. W. (2000). Image motion and context: A between and within subjects com- parison. Psychophysiology, 37, 706-710.
doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3750706
|
[46]
|
Tremoulet, P. D., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29, 943-951.
doi:10.1068/p3101
|
[47]
|
Troje, N. F., Westhoff, C., & Lavrov, M. (2005). Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 667-675. doi:10.3758/BF03193523
|
[48]
|
Troje, N. F., & Westhoff, C. (2006). The inversion effect in biological motion perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? Current Biology, 16, 821-824. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022
|
[49]
|
Wentura, D., Voss, A., & Rothermund, K. (2009). Playing TETRIS for science counter-regulatory affective processing in a motivationnally “hot” context. Acta Psychologica, 131, 171-177.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.05.008
|