[1]
|
Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 258-281. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.258
|
[2]
|
Anthony, J. S. (2004). Affection and attention: On the phenomenology of becoming aware. Continental Philosophy Review, 37, 21-43.
doi:10.1023/B:MAWO.0000049298.44397.be
|
[3]
|
Brooks, P. H., & McCauley, C. (1984). Cognitive research in mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 88, 479-486.
|
[4]
|
Craik, F. I., & Masani, P. A. (1969). Age and intelligence differences in coding and retrieval of word lists. British Journal of Psychology, 60, 315-319. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1969.tb01202.x
|
[5]
|
Crosby, K. G., & Blatt, B. (1968). Attention and mental retardation. Journal of Education, 150, 67-81.
|
[6]
|
Darwin, C. (1872). On the expression of emotions in men and animals. London: John Murray. doi:10.1037/10001-000
|
[7]
|
Denny, M. R. (1964). Learning and performance. In H. A. Stevens, & R. Heber (Eds.), Mental retardation: A review of research (pp. 100-142). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
|
[8]
|
Denny, M. R. (1966). A theoretical analysis and its application to training the retarded. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (pp. 1-27). New York: Academic Press.
doi:10.1016/S0074-7750(08)60201-3
|
[9]
|
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.
doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
|
[10]
|
Drew, C. J., Jardman, M., & Logan, D. R. (1996). Mental retardation. A life cycle approach (6th ed.). London: Prentice Hall.
|
[11]
|
Drew, C. J., Hardman, M. L., & Hart, A. W. (1996). Designing and conducting research in education and social science. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
|
[12]
|
Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Review Neuroscience and Psychology, 298, 1191-1194.
|
[13]
|
Goleman, D. P. (1995). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ for character, health and lifelong achievement. New York: Bantam Books.
|
[14]
|
Hadley, C. B., & Mackay, D. G. (2006). Does emotion help or hinder immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, Cognition, 32, 79-88. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.79
|
[15]
|
Hinojosa, J. A., Martín-Loeches, M., Fernández-Frías, C., & Rubia, F. J. (2001). Functional differences in the semantic processing of concrete and abstract words. Neuropsychologia, 39, 1086-1096.
doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00033-1
|
[16]
|
Howe, M. L., & Hunter, M. A. (1985). Adult age differences in storage-retrieval processes: A stages-of-learning analysis of developmental interactions in concreteness effects. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 130-150. doi:10.1037/h0080119
|
[17]
|
Hutt, M. L., & Gibby, R. G. (1965). The mentally retarded child (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
|
[18]
|
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 352-367.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.352
|
[19]
|
Luria, A. R. (1963). The mentally retarded child. Oxford: Pergamon.
|
[20]
|
Moore, D. G. (2001). Reassessing emotion recognition performance in people with mental retardation: A review. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106, 481-502.
doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2001)106<0481:RERPIP>2.0.CO;2
|
[21]
|
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
|
[22]
|
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.
|
[23]
|
Richardson, J. (2003). Dual coding versus relational processing in memory for concrete and abstract words. European Journal of Cognitive psychology, 15, 481-509. doi:10.1080/09541440244000256
|
[24]
|
Rojahn, J., Rabold, D. E., & Schneider, F. (1995). Emotion specificity in mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 477-486.
|
[25]
|
Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.
|
[26]
|
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 29-39. doi:10.1037/h0024127
|
[27]
|
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Stowe, R. W. (1989). Context availability and the processing of abstract and concrete words in sentences. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 114-126. doi:10.2307/748013
|
[28]
|
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220-247. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1
|
[29]
|
Talmi, D., Anderson, A. K., Riggs, L., Caplan, J. B., & Moscovitch M. (2007). Immediate memory consequences of the effect of emotion on attention to pictures. Learning and Memory, 15, 172-182.
doi:10.1101/lm.722908
|
[30]
|
Waring, J. D., Payne, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2010). Impact of individual differences upon emotion induced memory trade-offs. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 150-167. doi:10.1080/02699930802618918
|
[31]
|
Zeaman, D., & House, B. J. (1963). The role of attention in retardate discrimination learning. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), Handbook of mental deficiency (pp. 159-223). New York: McGraw-Hill.
|
[32]
|
Zigler, E. (1966). Personality structure in the retardate. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (pp. 77-108). New York: Academic Press.
|