(5 of 5) Autoethnography as a Research Strategy: Scientific? Of Course!!
As is, many esteemed academias look down on autoethnography as part of science, despite its clear inclusion as one of the research strategies in Saunders' Research Onion. Their skepticism often stems from their alignment with traditional scientific paradigms, particularly positivism and empiricism, which prioritize objective measurement, quantifiable data, and replicability. In these frameworks, knowledge is typically acquired through empirical observation, requiring detachment from the researcher’s personal experience. Autoethnography, by contrast, integrates personal narrative and reflection, challenging these established criteria.
Autoethnography is often viewed with skepticism by traditionalists in academia, particularly those who adhere to empirical or positivist approaches to research. This skepticism can lead some to argue that autoethnography is not scientific, based on its focus on personal narratives and subjectivity. However, this view is rooted in a narrow definition of what constitutes 'scientific' methodology.
Auguste Comte, a key figure in the development of Positivism, argued that the only authentic knowledge comes from observable, scientific evidence, building on the empirical methods of thinkers like Francis Bacon. Comte's ideas contributed to the belief that scientific knowledge based solely on empirical observation is the only valid form of understanding.
This viewpoint, however, has been widely criticized for its arrogance in dismissing other sources of knowledge. Karl Popper, one of Comte's most notable critics, rejected the idea that science should be limited to observable facts. Popper argued that scientific theories cannot be conclusively proven by observation alone but must be open to falsification, emphasizing that progress comes through continuous testing and refutation. Similarly, Immanuel Kant opposed the positivist framework by demonstrating that knowledge is also shaped by innate mental structures, not just sensory data. Thomas Kuhn further challenged Comte’s rigid view by showing that scientific progress involves shifts in paradigms, rather than the linear accumulation of facts as Positivism suggested.
Empiricism, rooted in Positivism, is only one of the many methods through which we acquire knowledge. Positivism is a philosophical stance that relies on observable, empirical evidence to generate knowledge, which aligns with the principles of empiricism. There are 11 others alongside it including philosophical reflection, historical analysis, artistic interpretation, intuition, belief and doubt, skepticism etc. Each offers unique insights into different aspects of human understanding. Therefore, it is essential to approach knowledge acquisition with humility, recognizing that no single method holds all the answers.
In reality, autoethnography employs rigorous methodologies, triangulation for objectivity, and a reflective process that make it a legitimate scientific method of inquiry.
This skepticism overlooks the fact that autoethnography is scientifically valid within interpretivist, constructivist, and postmodern paradigms—approaches that recognize multiple realities and the complexity of human experience. While traditionalists in academia may argue that autoethnography's focus on personal narratives disqualifies it from being scientific, this narrow view of 'science' fails to account for the rigor, triangulation, and reflexivity embedded in the autoethnographic methodology. Thus, autoethnography stands as a legitimate scientific method of inquiry, broadening the definition of what constitutes scientific research.
1. Revisiting Autoethnography
Autoethnography is a qualitative research method that combines autobiography and ethnography, where the researcher uses personal experiences to examine cultural, social, and political contexts. Through the lens of personal narrative, the autoethnographer connects their own lived experiences with larger social phenomena, creating a deeply introspective form of research that simultaneously addresses individual and collective understanding.
The method emphasizes self-reflection, subjectivity, and the interplay between the personal and the cultural. This does not negate its scientific nature, but rather broadens the definition of science to include interpretive, reflective methodologies that do not rely solely on empirical data.
Writer Joan Didion notes simply and powerfully, “we tell stories in order to live.” (Adams et al, 2015)
.........
Autoethnographic stories are artistic and analytic demonstrations of how we come to know, name, and interpret personal and cultural experience. With autoethnography, we use our experience to engage ourselves, others, culture(s), politics, and social research. In doing autoethnography, we confront “the tension between insider and outsider perspectives, between social practice and social constraint. (Adams et al, 2015)
Hence, autoethnography is a research method that: Uses a researcher’s personal experience to describe and critique cultural beliefs, practices, and experiences; Acknowledges and values a researcher’s relationships with others; Uses deep and careful self-reflection—typically referred to as “reflexivity”—to name and interrogate the intersections between self and society, the particular and the general, the personal and the political; Shows “people in the process of figuring out what to do, how to live, and the meaning of their struggles; Balances intellectual and methodological rigor, emotion, and creativity; Strives for social justice and to make life better. (Adams et al, 2015)
2. Why Autoethnography Is Scientific
While autoethnography may not align with empirical or positivist traditions, it is scientific in its own right for several key reasons:
Recommended by LinkedIn
3. Addressing Common Criticisms
The primary criticism leveled against autoethnography is that it is too subjective and personal to be considered scientific. However, subjectivity does not necessarily contradict scientific inquiry. Autoethnography acknowledges the researcher’s role as a participant in their own research, offering a different kind of objectivity—one that is grounded in transparency and reflexivity.
Other common criticisms include the following:
4. "Subjectivity as My Objectivity"
One of the hallmarks of autoethnography is the claim that “subjectivity is my objectivity.” This does not mean that personal experience is immune to bias or critique, but rather that the researcher’s subjective perspective is a legitimate and valuable source of knowledge. In autoethnography, subjectivity is seen as a tool for gaining deeper insights into cultural, social, and psychological phenomena.
The idea is that by analyzing personal experience through rigorous reflection and theoretical frameworks, the researcher produces a form of objectivity that acknowledges and incorporates subjectivity as part of the research process. This challenges the traditional notion of objectivity, but it does so in a way that enhances the richness and depth of understanding.
5. Autoethnography as a Challenge to Traditional Definitions of Science
Autoethnography expands the definition of science. It challenges the dominance of positivism and empiricism by demonstrating that valid, reliable knowledge can be acquired through reflective, interpretive methods. Autoethnography invites a rethinking of what it means to engage in scientific inquiry, urging us to consider multiple ways of knowing.
In this sense, autoethnography is not "unscientific"; rather, it highlights the limitations of traditional scientific paradigms. By embracing personal narratives, reflexivity, and subjective experience, autoethnography offers a powerful alternative to more conventional research methodologies, contributing unique insights to the broader scientific community.
TAKEAWAY
Autoethnography has been recognized as a legitimate scientific method since the 1980s and 1990s, gaining traction particularly through the work of scholars like Carolyn Ellis and Arthur Bochner. By the early 2000s, it had established itself as a credible qualitative research method in fields such as anthropology, sociology, and cultural studies, where personal narrative and cultural analysis were combined to offer deep insights. However, resistance persists in disciplines grounded in positivism and empiricism, which prioritize quantifiable data, replicability, and objective detachment. These biases narrow the understanding of what constitutes valid research, often overlooking the significant contributions of subjective and reflexive methods. Autoethnography challenges these limitations by broadening the scope of scientific inquiry and demonstrating that rigorous knowledge can emerge from diverse epistemological approaches, enriching our understanding of human experience and the world.
REFERENCES:
Adams, T. E., Holman Jones, S., & Ellis, C. (2015). Autoethnography: Understanding Qualitative Research. Oxford University Press.